The Perceptual Study of the Tolerance of Spectral Images Based on Bootstrap Analysis


Article Preview

Spectral images contain a large volume of data and place considerable demands on computer hardware and software compared with standard trichromatic image storage and processing. Although the imformation of reflectance spectra may be represented efficiently using linear models a key task is to answer how many basis functions of a linear model are necessary for a given accuracy of representation. Because the most common application of spectral images is the reproduction of color and images, it is highly important to study the tolerance of human perceptions over spectral images represented by low-dimensional linear models. In this study several data sets and psychophysical studies have been used to investigate how many basis functions are necessary to represent a spectral image to be indistinguishable from the original image. The raw objective experiment datas are analysed by a comprehensive probit analysis based on the bootstrap method, and results show that the perceptual tolerance for spectral images reconsctucted by variouse number of basis functions is highly image dependent.



Advanced Materials Research (Volumes 301-303)

Edited by:

Riza Esa and Yanwen Wu




Q. Chen et al., "The Perceptual Study of the Tolerance of Spectral Images Based on Bootstrap Analysis", Advanced Materials Research, Vols. 301-303, pp. 1151-1156, 2011

Online since:

July 2011




[1] Maloney, L. T. (1986). Evaluation of linear models of surface spectral reflectances with small numbers of parameters. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, 3, 1673-1683.

[2] Maloney, L. T. (1999). Physics-based approaches to modeling surface color perception, Cambridge University Press.

[3] Romero, J., Garcia-Beltran, A. and Hermandez-Andrez, J. (1997). Linear bases for representation of natural and artificial illuminants. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, 14, 1007-1014.

DOI: 10.1364/josaa.14.001007

[4] Owens, H. C. (2002). Color and spatiochromatic processing in the human visual system. PHD Thesis, University of Derby.

[5] Parkkinen, J., Hallikainen, J. and Jääskeläinen, T. (1989). Charateristic spectra of Munsell colors. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, 6, 378-322.

[6] Jaaskelainen, T., Parkkinen, J. and Toyooka, S. (1990). Vector-subspace model for color representation. Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, 7(4), 725-730.

DOI: 10.1364/josaa.7.000725

[7] Hardeberg, J. Y. (2002). On the spectral dimensionality of object colours. In Proceedings of the First European Conference on Colour in Graphics, Imaging, and Vision, 480-485.

[8] Q. Chen, L. Wang, , S. Westland., A Perceptual Study of Linear Models of Spectral Reflectance. ppt. Proc. Of the 2nd International Conference on Graphic Communications, IEEE Computer Society, 2008, 309-312.

[9] Finney, D. J. (1971). Probit analysis, 3rd edition. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

[10] Efron, B. (1979). Bootstrap methods: Another look at the jackknife. Annals of Statistics, 7, 1-26.

[11] Foster, D. H. and Bischof, W. F. (1991). Thresholds from psychometric functions: Superiority of bootstrap to incremental and probit variance estimators. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 152-159.

DOI: 10.1037//0033-2909.109.1.152

[12] Leek, M. R. (2001). Adaptive procedures in psychophysical research. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(8), 1279-1292.

DOI: 10.3758/bf03194543

[13] Wichmann, F. A. and Hill, N. J. (2001). The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling and goodness-of-fit. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(8), 1293-1313.

DOI: 10.3758/bf03194544

Fetching data from Crossref.
This may take some time to load.