[1]
J. V. Stone, N. M. Hunkin, J. Porrill, R. Wood, V. Keeler, M. Beanland, M. Port and N. R. Porter (2001) When is now? Perception of simultaneity, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268 31-38.
DOI: 10.1098/rspb.2000.1326
Google Scholar
[2]
C. Spence, and S. Squire (2003) Multisensory integration: Maintaining the Perception of Synchrony, Current Biology 13: 13 R519-R521.
DOI: 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00445-7
Google Scholar
[3]
L.R. Harris, V. Harrar, P.M. Jaekl, and A. Kopinska (2009) Mechanisms of simultaneity constancy. In: Nijhawan R (ed) Issues of space and time in perception and action. Cambridge University Press.
DOI: 10.1017/cbo9780511750540.015
Google Scholar
[4]
D. Alais and S. Carlile. (2005). Synchronizing to real events: subjective audiovisual alignment scales with perceived auditory depth and speed of sound., Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 2244–2247.
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0407034102
Google Scholar
[5]
W. Fujisaki, S. Shimojo, M. Kashino, and S. Nishida (2004) Recalibration of audio–visual simultaneity., Nat. Neurosci. 7: 773–778.
DOI: 10.1038/nn1268
Google Scholar
[6]
K. P Körding, U. Beierholm, W.J. Ma, S. Quartz, J.B. Tenenbaum et al. (2007) Causal Inference in Multisensory Perception., PLoS ONE 2(9): e943.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000943
Google Scholar
[7]
G. H Recanzone (2003) Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception, J. Neurophysiol 89 1078–1093.
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00706.2002
Google Scholar
[8]
J. Vroomen, and B. de Gelder (2004) Temporal Ventriloquism: Sound Modulates the Flash-Lag Effect., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30: 3 513-518.
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.513
Google Scholar
[9]
Y. Wada, N. Kitagawa, and K. Noguchi (2003) Audio-visual integration in temporal perception, International Journal of Psychophysiology 50: 1-2 117-124.
DOI: 10.1016/s0167-8760(03)00128-4
Google Scholar
[10]
W. J Ma, and A. Pouget (2008) Linking neurons to behaviour in multisensory perception: a computational review, Brain Research 10: 1016.
Google Scholar
[11]
S. Deneve, and A. Pouget (2004) Bayesian multisensory integration and cross-modal spatial links, Journal of Physiology –Paris 98 249-258.
DOI: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.03.011
Google Scholar
[12]
U. Beierholm, K. Kording, L. Shams, and W.J. Ma, (2007). Comparing Bayesian models for multisensory cue combination without mandatory integration., In: Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems.
Google Scholar
[13]
M. Keetels , and J. Vroomen (2009) Perception of synchrony between the senses. In M. M. Murray & M. T. Wallace (Eds. ), Frontiers in the neural basis of multisensory processes.
DOI: 10.1201/b11092-12
Google Scholar
[14]
A. Vatakis and C. Spence (2007) Evaluating the influence of the 'unity assumption' on the temporal perception of realistic audiovisual stimuli, B. Acta Psychologica 127: 1 12-23.
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.12.002
Google Scholar
[15]
Baumann and Greenlee (2007) Neural correlates of coherent audiovisual motion perception, Cerebral Cortex 17: 1433-1443.
DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhl055
Google Scholar
[16]
G.F. Meyer and S. M Wuerger (2001) Cross-modal integration of auditory and visual motion signals, Neuroreport 12: 118 2557-2560.
DOI: 10.1097/00001756-200108080-00053
Google Scholar
[17]
C. Parise, and C. Spence. (2008). Synesthetic congruency modulates the temporal ventriloquism effect., Neuroscience Letters, 442, 257–261.
DOI: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.010
Google Scholar
[18]
C. Parise, and C. Spence. (2009). When birds of a feather flock together: Synesthetic correspondences modulate audiovisual integration in non-synesthetes., PLoS ONE, 4, e5664.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005664
Google Scholar
[19]
J. Vroomen and M. Keetels (2011) No effect of synesthetic congruency on temporal ventriloquism., Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 73 209-218.
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-010-0019-0
Google Scholar
[20]
N. Naber, T Carlson, F. Verstraten, W. Einhauser (2011) Perceptual benefits of objecthood, Journal of Vision 11(4): 8, 1-9.
DOI: 10.1167/11.4.8
Google Scholar
[21]
L. Busse, K. Roberts, R. Crist, D. Weissman, M. Woldorff (2005) The spread of attention across modalities and space in a multisensory object, Proceedings of the Nat. ional Academy of Sciences 102: 51 18751-18756.
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0507704102
Google Scholar
[22]
J McDonald, W. Teder-Salejarvi, F. Di Russo, and S. Hillyard (2003) Neural substrates of perceptual enchancement by cross-modal spatial attention, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15: 1 10-19.
DOI: 10.1162/089892903321107783
Google Scholar
[23]
L. Marks, R Hammeal, and M. Bornstein (1987) Children's comprehension of cross-modal similarity: perception and metaphor., Monographs in social research and child development 42: 11-91.
Google Scholar
[24]
Marks (2004) Cross-modal interactions in speeded classification In The handbook of multisensory processes, Calvert, Spence, and Stein (Eds) MIT Press (2004).
DOI: 10.7551/mitpress/3422.003.0009
Google Scholar
[25]
R. Malera (1989) Dimensional interaction between color and pitch, Journal of Experimantal Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15: 1 69-79.
Google Scholar
[26]
H. Levitt (1971) Transformed up‐down methods in psychoacoustics., J Acoust Soc Am 1971; 49 Suppl 2: 467–76.
Google Scholar
[27]
R. Alcala-Quintana, and M. Garcia-Perez (2007) A comparison of fixed-step-size and Bayesian staircases for sensory threshold estimation, Spatial Vision, 20: 3 197-218.
DOI: 10.1163/156856807780421174
Google Scholar
[28]
D. Slutsky, and G. Recanzone (2001) Temporal and spatial dependency of the ventriloquism effect, Auditory and Vestibular Systems 12: 1 7-10.
DOI: 10.1097/00001756-200101220-00009
Google Scholar
[29]
G. Recanzone (2002) Auditory influences on visual temporal rate perception, Journal of Neurophysiology 89: 2 1078-1093.
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00706.2002
Google Scholar